What do tickles tell us about the brain?
Interview with
Building a tickle machine: a robot, holding a feather duster, to find out why people with schizophrenia can tickle themselves.
Now, during the show, we've been investigating laughter and one of the things that makes most of us giggle is being tickled. [Tickle recording]. The power of tickle. Now, some people are ubber ticklish whilst others don't react at all to a gentle tactile tease. What's more, it's almost impossible to tickle yourself. That is unless you have the psychiatric illness Schizophrenia. In order to investigate this, Professor Daniel Wolpert from Cambridge University built himself a tickle machine - the equivalent of a robot holding a feather duster. I met with him, firstly asking him what tickling can tell us about how our brains work.
Daniel - The reason we were interested in tickling is that it's a very nice example of how the brain does sensory processing. So, one feature you have to do when you process information that comes in through your visual system through hearing or through touch is that, some of the sensations you get are due to what you're doing with your own body. Some of it just comes about passively from things moving around in the outside world.
The fundamental thing the brain wants to determine is, what bits of the sensory input I'm getting come from external source and what comes from what I do. Tickling is a very nice example where when you do something to yourself, it feels very different from when somebody else does. And so, there were hypotheses from '50s which suggested that perhaps tickling had to do with the fact that brain make predictions of what you were going to feel based on your own actions and remove that off. Just in the way those noise cancelling headphones work. On airplanes, they cancel off the external sounds. You hear the internal sound coming from your speakers, but the brain would have to make a prediction, rather complex prediction of what was going to happen.
To study this, we set up a really nice robotic interface which allow us to control the relationship to what you do and what you feel. So, you'd hold a stick in one hand attached to a robot with 3 dimensions and we could track that movement. You just control another robot which tickles you. And so, we could replicate an old '70s study where either you move them to tickle yourself or the robot will just tickle you without movement, and what we found is what we'd expect. It felt much less ticklish when you tickled yourself compared to the robot tickling you. But the really nice thing we could do is put in some different perturbations. So, we started to remove back and forward with the tickling stick and we replay to themselves in tickle mode, but with time plays of 2/10 of a second. With these very small time plays, the tickling sensation became greater and greater. By 3/10 of a second time delay, it felt like the rubber which is tickling you were much more ticklish than you were doing it yourself. So, it seemed that temporal causality was very important for cancelling off this sensation.
Hannah - And what happens in for example, Schizophrenia where people with Schizophrenia tickle themselves?
Daniel - So, there was prominent theory of some of the delusions that Schizophrenic patients have. For example, they have something called delusions of control. That means they make normal movements, but they claim their movements are being controlled by external sources. And so, this predictive phenomena can actually be used for a different thing. They need to cancel off sensory feedback, but also to determine whether the movements they're making are due to me or due to external source. So, the moment I can hear a voice speaking in this room, but I'm pretty sure it's my voice speaking in the room rather than you speaking, or my voice being played back to me. One way you can do that is I can make a prediction based on what I'm doing to my vocal tract and to my lungs or the sounds I should hear. If those match well with what I do hear, it's me doing it. On the other hand, if I hear my voice being played back, I'll realise it's not me because I won't be able to make the predictions of what I should hear. And so, one theory of delusions of control is that Schizophrenic patients can have is that they fail in terms of their prediction. They're surprised by their own movements because they didn't predict the consequences and therefore, it should be them to external sources. Consequence of this is that effectively, they don't have the predictive ability and therefore, there should have no difference in their tickle ability compared to self tickle and external tickle. Although I wasn't involved in the study, a group of patients were examined up in Edinburgh and it found exactly that they didn't show any difference in their perception of self tickle compared to external tickle.
Hannah - So, they can effectively tickle themselves.
Daniel - More likely that they can tickle themselves compared to normal people.
Hannah - What else have your tickle machine has been telling you?
Daniel - Well, based on the tickle study, we actually went on to look at some other things. So, one thing we were very interested in, I have two children who, at that time of that study were quite young. And what children on back seats of cars on long journeys, they tend to get into these fights which turns with one of them does one thing to another, the other one responds and it quickly escalates to the point at which you scream at them. My two daughters would get into these fights and often claim, both of them, the other person hit them harder. I feel as a neuroscientist, it was my job to explain because I knew my children never lie, to how they can be telling inconsistent truths. And so, we thought from the tickling study, we can partly explain why these sorts of fights tend to escalate. And that's because when you actively do something to someone else, because you generate the commands to do it, you predict the sensory feedback and subtract it off. So if one child hits another, they think they've hit the child or person less hard. So, if the other child retaliates with the same force, the first child thinks it's been escalated. So, based on that observation, we decided to run these sorts of studies and not in children hitting, but with adults and just gentle touching, in a tit for tat type game. What we found is when got people to play the tit for tat type game where one person touch another and they have to touch back with the same force, if they don't know the rules of a person that's playing and that we brief the subjects in different rooms, we find the forces they use escalate rapidly over the course of 5 or 6 turns each. If you asked the people what rule just the other person was playing by, they all things like, well, they're being asked to double it. So, we are now showing that when you generate forces on the outside world, you always tend to underestimate the forces that you're generating to this general predictive phenomena. And we think that's true not only of forces, but also in terms of speech. We always think as being quieter than we really are and so on. It's a general feature of attenuating what we believe we're doing on the outside world.
Hannah - How does this fit in with patients with Schizophrenia?
Daniel - Well, the prediction from this is a nice prediction. So, in normal subjects, when they feel a force passing, they have to reproduce it actively. They push too hard because they attenuate their own forces. Since we believe there is a failure with this predictive mechanism in patients with Schizophrenia then effectively, we predict they should be better at that task. So, if you don't attenuate it should be better at matching forces. The nice thing about that is, mostly in patient studies, you assume the patient could be worse or something and here, we have a prediction they could be better. We've tested this out in a group of patients who have Schizophrenia and age and IQ match controls. So, what we found is there's significantly better than controls in that when they experience a force passively, they have to match it actively by pushing with their finger, they do a much better job than normal subjects. Interestingly, we see quite a variety of attenuation in normal people. So, some people attenuate a lot when they produce forces, some don't attenuate very much. And we recently had a study in Cambridge where we gave people a delusional inventory questionnaire. So these are just normal undergraduates in Cambridge. Questions such as, do you ever feel you're destined for greatness? And because we're in Cambridge, many people have delusions they're destined for greatness and that's part of a delusional thinking.
Hannah - I was about to say, can you get a normal volunteer, amongst the Cambridge students?
Daniel - We get a range. The questions go from, do you ever think people are talking about you behind your back? Do you ever thought you were a zombie but no will of your own? And so, from this, we can get a measure on each subject of their delusional thinking. What we found is it strongly correlates with how much they attenuate. So, even with normal population, somehow delusional gradations of thinking correlate with how much you attenuate when you're joining these force matching tasks. Well, the more delusional you are, the less you attenuate, so the more effect you have on the world around you.
The basic experiment is very simple. People put their finger under a small leaver which is attached to a little motor and the motor generates a small force on the finger and then stops. The subject's task is to use that other hand to push another leaver from above to generate the same person. And so, what they do is they tend to push too hard because when they do it themselves, they'd predict what's going to happen subtract it off to push harder. They push harder about 50%. So if they experience a force of a kilo, they will produce 1 ½ kilos of force to get the same experience.
Hannah - So, people that are delusional, they actually have a very accurate self representation of their effect in the world around them?
Daniel - That's correct. So, people with Schizophrenia who have delusions of control, they are able to match accurately because they don't have the predictive phenomena and therefore, they can actually match more accurately than normal subjects.
Hannah - So, are they delusional?
Daniel - Well, unfortunately, yes because what it means is they find it hard to separate out with things that they've done in the world from things that just happen in the world. So, you need this predictive phenomena to be able to know, is it me or is it someone else which is doing something? And they're not able to disambiguate between those two, even though they can match forces more accurately.
Hannah - Thanks to Daniel Wolpert from Cambridge University. He probes delusions using tickle and tapping machines to get to grips with why people with Schizophrenia seem better equipped at tickling themselves. It's due to the problem of distinguishing whether sensations arise from one own actions or from external sources.
So, most healthy volunteers can identify sensation that they cause themselves and then discount or attenuate them. Hence, our inability to tickle ourselves.
However, some patients with Schizophrenia seem to lack the ability to attenuate the sensations arising from their own actions, which leads them to showing little difference in their perception of external and self tickle.
- Previous Visiting a Laughter Clinic
- Next How does humour develop?
Comments
Add a comment